top of page
Mariya Derevytska

The Hidden Force: Exploring Framing Bias in Economic and Financial contexts

Updated: Dec 7, 2024


Importance of understanding decision-making:


Our choices shape the world, within which financial and economic systems play a

crucial role. Yet, the significance of decision-making is often dismissed as ‘common

sense,’ a simplification that traditional economic and financial theories once

overlooked.


Predicting the decisions of others to anticipate investment trends requires a nuanced

understanding that transcends surface-level models like traditional game theory or

historical stock performance analysis. Even an innocuous compliment can tap into

an investor’s mood - a key factor in risk perception. Lowenstein et al., 2001 showed

that positive moods can reduce risk aversion, potentially leading to more aggressive

investment behaviour, highlighting how subconscious biases and emotional states

interplay with rational strategies, shaping the market in ways that established

theories often fail to capture.


However, in the real world our emotional state does not work in isolation. It is a mix

of factors, where the way information is presented is equally pivotal. Framing the

same financial opportunity in terms of gains rather than losses has a significant

potential of exploiting our brain’s reliance on heuristics. While the heuristic

mechanism has evolved to help us make quick decisions and simplify our lives, it

now has a different effect on the contemporary financial market environment and

consumer behaviour. For example, in consumer markets, the anchoring heuristic

distorts judgment by concentrating focus on an initial reference point, even when the

framing of the outcome is changed. This can lead to an overestimation of benefits,

such as when consumers prefer discounts framed as a percentage (e.g., 50% off)

rather than a flat price reduction (e.g., $50 off), because the relative value of the

discount appears larger when expressed as a percentage, even though the

monetary value is identical.



Prospect theory vs Expected Utility Theory:


Prospect theory explains framing bias, suggesting that people value gains and

losses differently, leading to decisions driven more by the risk of loss than the

potential for gain. According to this theory, people are more likely to take risks to

avoid losses than to achieve gains, which directly contradicts the basic rational

assumptions of the underlying economic theories. For example, The Expected Utility

Theory assumes that utility is quantifiable (if A>B and B>C, then A>C) and decisions

are based on the consideration of probability outcomes; suggesting that individuals

prefer a guaranteed outcome over a probabilistic one with a higher expected utility. It

thereby suggests that introduction of alternative choices will have no effect on

decisions, which does not accurately reflect the actual outcomes. This set of

independence axiom assumptions has been increasingly criticised by the pioneers of

behavioural economics. Simon, 1955 pointed out the common choice inconsistence

in the real-world scenarios, raising a question about the transitive preferences

suggested by the Expected Utility theory.


The groundbreaking findings of 2 Israeli psychologists Tversky and Kahneman in

1981, have led to the Prospect Theory creation and opened the doors into the filed of

behavioural science, promoting more emphasis on cognitive biases. In their

research, participants had to choose between two treatments for 600 people afflicted

with a fatal disease. The first treatment would result in 400 deaths, while the second

had a 66% chance of everyone dying and a 33% chance of no one dying. Of the

participants, 72% chose the first option when it was positively framed, but only 22%

chose it when it was negatively framed. This research has clearly showed the effect

of positive framing on decisions, even in life-or-death situations.



Behavioural Challenges to traditional financial models:


The Capital Asset Pricing Model (CAPM) is a widely used framework in the financial

sector that provides insights into how an asset’s expected return can be calculated

based on its beta coefficient, which measures its sensitivity to overall market

movements. CAPM assumes that investors are only concerned with systematic risk,

the portion of risk that cannot be diversified away. According to this model, the

Efficient Market Hypothesis (EMH) posits that all available information is already

reflected in stock prices, implying that markets are efficient, and investors cannot

consistently achieve returns greater than the market average.


However, these assumptions have been increasingly criticised, particularly when

considering the role of psychological and emotional factors in decision-making.

Framing bias is one such factor that can significantly alter investor behaviour,

causing deviations from the rational decisions predicted by CAPM. For instance,

when risks are framed as losses rather than gains, investors tend to exhibit greater

risk aversion, even if the underlying probabilities remain unchanged. In asset

markets, such biases can lead to overreactions or underreactions to new information

and market events, contrasting with the rational actor assumption of the EMH.

Consequently, the possibility of “beating the market” becomes more plausible than

the EMH suggests when cognitive biases are taken into account, challenging the

assumption that investors always act to maximise utility.


To address these shortcomings, the Behavioural Asset Pricing Model (BAPM) has

been developed. Unlike traditional models, BAPM acknowledges that investors often

make decisions that deviate from pure rationality. It incorporates psychological

factors like emotions, overconfidence, and heuristics such as anchoring bias,

providing a more nuanced and realistic understanding of investor behaviour and

asset pricing in real-world financial markets.



Effect of Framing Bias on Public Opinions:


Brexit referendum is one of the clearest real-life examples of the effect of

framing bias on political decisions, public opinions, and consequently

economic state of the UK. The proponents of leaving the EU often employed

framing techniques to emphasise the potential losses associated with

remaining, rather than focusing on gains. The use of loss aversion was their

dominant strategy, involving the emphasis on what the UK stood to lose - such

as sovereignty, control over borders, or financial contributions to the EU

despite there being strong economic ties especially in trade and investments,

that have contributed to the UK’s stability in the aftermath of the 2008 financial

crisis. Phrases like “taking back control” or “protecting British jobs” tapped into

voters’ emotional reactions to the perceived loss of national autonomy,

exploiting their fear of loss. This heightened sensitivity to modern ‘threats’ is

believed to have evolved as a survival mechanism, making individuals more

motivated to avoid harm than to pursue equivalent benefits. Brexit proponents

successfully triggered these evolutionary responses, leading voters to

prioritise avoiding the perceived losses of EU membership over the ‘uncertain’

benefits of remaining.



Role of Framing Bias in Real-life Events:


In times of uncertainty, framing effect is even more pronounced, influencing asset

allocation. While equities and bonds typically offer higher returns, many investors

shift toward safe-harbour assets: government bonds, gold, or money market funds

during periods of financial instability. These investments, which are seen as low risk,

provide stability despite lower returns. This shift is often driven by framing the current

market conditions as “risky” or “uncertain,” which pushes investors to prioritise

security over returns, even when potential gains may outweigh the risk. This same

mechanism can also fuel speculative bubbles, which the Dot-Com Bubbles of the

late 1990s reflects. In this case, investors were overconfident, driven by the fear of

missing out and excitement over latest technology, and influenced by herd mentality

where investors mirrored the actions of others, pushing prices up further. During this

bubble framing has significantly contributed to positive wealth effect, which increased

investor optimism leading to over investment. The media was also a crucial factor in

framing tech stocks as revolutionary and essential for the future. This led to investors

building up those stocks, but when the reality did not meet expectations, the bubble

burst, causing major market declines.


Another more recent example is the Cryptocurrency Bubble of 2017, driven by the

rapid rise of Bitcoin specifically, as it was framed as revolutionary blockchain

technology, fuelling speculative investment. Over time, market corrections occurred

due to both natural market forces, such as the rapid decline in the value of Bitcoin

and Ethereum which brought prices back to sustainable levels; and government

regulations, such as licensing requirements and trading restrictions with a

discouragement purpose.


Another striking example of how framing bias can exacerbate economic

instability is the Northern Rock bank run of 2007.The media intensified the

bank’s liquidity issues by using sensationalist language, triggering widespread

panic. This framing led to a rush of depositors withdrawing their funds, fearing

the loss of their savings. The ensuing panic led to the bank’s collapse, which,

in turn, contributed significantly to the broader financial crisis, severely

undermining economic stability.



Importance of acknowledging framing bias:


Framing bias is far from a subtle influence - it is a powerful force shaping

decision-making across the variety of fields. Whether it is influencing the

investment choices of seasoned traders or swaying the political beliefs of

electorate majority, framing bias impacts how we perceive risk, reward from

insights provided by data. From Brexit to the Dot-Com and Cryptocurrency

Bubbles, framing bias has proven time and again to be a powerful tool for

those who understand its influence. By acknowledging its effect on everything

from asset pricing models to consumer behaviour, we not only enhance our

ability to predict market trends but also make more rational choices in the

increasingly unpredictable world.

21 views0 comments

Recent Posts

See All

Commentaires


bottom of page